Probability of getting a second $6$

A sister has two fair six sided dice, one red one blue, the dice are rolled together. The sister announces to her brother who is sat in another room and unable to see the dice, that there is at least one six in the outcome. Then asks him what are the odds that the other die is a six. He replies $1/6$, she says no it has to be $1/11$. Who is right or most likely to be right? Please help settle this family feud!


In two ways:

  1. We are computing the probability that both dice are a $6$ given all of the information he knows, not her. It is worth noting that she can see the other die, so she knows with certainty (probability $1$) whether both are $6$s or not.

    She rolls both dice at the same time. There are 11 pairs of dice for which her statement that "at least one of the dice are a $6$" is true, i.e., $$(1,6),(2,6),(3,6),(4,6),(5,6),(6,6),(6,1),(6,2),(6,3),(6,4),(6,5)$$ we are not told which of the dice was a $6$, so we cannot cancel any possibilities (which is how he incorrectly concluded $1/6$).

    Only one of these 11 are are both $6$s, hence the probability is $1/11$.

  2. In a similar vein, but more algebraic, let $X_2=I[\text{die 1 is $6$}]$ and $X_1=I[\text{die 2 is $6$}]$, then

    $$ \mathbb{P}[X_1+X_2=2 \,|\, X_1+X_2\geq 1] = \frac{\mathbb{P}[X_1+X_2 = 2]}{\mathbb{P}[X_1+X_2\geq 1]} = \frac{\mathbb{P}[X_1=1,X_2=1]}{1 - \mathbb{P}[X_1=0,X_2=0]}$$

    by independence and denoting $\mathbb{P}[X_1=1]=p_1$, $\mathbb{P}[X_2=1]=p_2$, \begin{align}= \frac{\mathbb{P}[X_1=1]\mathbb{P}[X_2=1]}{1 - \mathbb{P}[X_1=0]\mathbb{P}[X_2=0]}= \frac{p_1p_2}{1-(1-p_1)(1-p_2)} = \frac{1}{\tfrac{1}{p_1}+\tfrac{1}{p_2}-1}\end{align} Note that this allows for biased dice (e.g., what if the first die were weighted to land on $6$ 90% of the time?). But if we assume they are not biased, then $p_1=p_2=\tfrac{1}{6} \implies \tfrac{1}{p_1} = \tfrac{1}{p_2}=6$, giving the same result of $\frac{1}{(6)+(6)-1} = \frac{1}{11}$.

    We could then answer the same question if we replaced dice with coins ($p_1=p_2=\tfrac{1}{2}$), which gives $\tfrac{1}{(2)(2)-1}=\tfrac{1}{3}$.

Edit If the problem were different and instead she doesn't see the second die then the probability that she would guess correctly is $1/6$. If he knew that she had only seen one die then the probability he would be correct in his guess is $1/6$, but if he didn't have that information then it would still be $1/11$.

The confusion here is very similar to the Monty Hall problem.


This question can't be properly answered from the information provided (which is why it's so good at provoking arguments). To give a proper answer, you would need to know on what basis the sister decided what fact about the dice to announce. Suppose she thought to herself "I will look at the red die, which will be showing a number, $n$. I'll shout to my brother there is at least one $n$ in the outcome". She does this and it happens to be a 6 that comes up on the red die. All this faffing around with the red die obviously doesn't affect what number the blue die has landed on (the sister might not even have looked at it), so the probability that the other die is a 6 too is clearly 1/6.

To get an answer of 1/11 you need to make a similarly unjustified assumption about the sister's thought process (something like "If at least one die shows a 6 then I'll announce that fact, otherwise I won't say anything, and no maths question will be produced").

This is essentially the same as the "Tuesday Boy problem" as discussed by Rob Eastaway


BLUF: If this happened in real life, I would expect the brother to more likely be right (but for the wrong reason). But I imagine the person who posed the question to you had in mind a scenario where the sister is more likely to be right.


This is a standard problem, whose main issue isn't even the formal mathematics — it's in the modeling of the problem. Specifically, the problem gives insufficient information to specify what's going on, leaving the listener to fill in their own ideas about what problem they want to solve.

It's further compounded by a grammar error — nowhere in the problem has a die been specified, so it makes no sense to speak of the "other" die. This error tends to have a very strong effect of tricking people into a particular interpretation of the problem.

I'm going to resolve the grammar issue by changing the problem slightly, my change in bold:

... The sister announces to her brother who is sat in another room and unable to see the dice, that there is at least one six in the outcome. Then asks him what are the odds that both dice are six. ...


That said, I expect the brother to most likely be right. I expect the typical sister that does this would have gone through something resembling the following process:

  • She thinks "I just learned this neat probability thing. I'll see if I can get one up on my brother."
  • She rolls two dice
  • She (uniformly randomly) picks one of the two dice, and announces its value to her brother
  • She then asks the probability question

The third bullet point is very important. The point being, for example, that the outcome of seeing two dice with $\{6, 3\}$ happens with probability $\frac{1}{18}$, but the outcome that the dice were $\{ 6, 3\}$ and the sister chooses to announce the $6$ rather than the $3$ happens with probability $\frac{1}{36}$.

So, from the information the sister announces, the relevant outcomes are:

  • With probability $1/36$, the dice read $\{ 6, 1 \}$ and the sister announces $6$
  • With probability $1/36$, the dice read $\{ 6, 2 \}$ and the sister announces $6$
  • With probability $1/36$, the dice read $\{ 6, 3 \}$ and the sister announces $6$
  • With probability $1/36$, the dice read $\{ 6, 4 \}$ and the sister announces $6$
  • With probability $1/36$, the dice read $\{ 6, 5 \}$ and the sister announces $6$
  • With probability $1/36$, the dice read $\{ 6, 6 \}$ and the sister announces $6$

So, given the information that the sister announced a $6$, you find that the probability that both dice read $6$ is, indeed, $1/6$.


Now, another example of how this could have gone — and probably the one that the question's originator had in mind, is that the sister did something more like

  • She repeatedly rolls two dice until at least one of the two dice read $6$
  • She announces there is a $6$ and asks the question

In this case, the outcome that the dice read $\{ 6,1 \}$ upon announcement really is $1/18$. The relevant outcomes are

  • With probability $1/18$, the dice read $\{ 6, 1 \}$ when the announcement is made
  • With probability $1/18$, the dice read $\{ 6, 2 \}$ when the announcement is made
  • With probability $1/18$, the dice read $\{ 6, 3 \}$ when the announcement is made
  • With probability $1/18$, the dice read $\{ 6, 4 \}$ when the announcement is made
  • With probability $1/18$, the dice read $\{ 6, 5 \}$ when the announcement is made
  • With probability $1/36$, the dice read $\{ 6, 6 \}$ when the announcement is made

and we see the probability is $\frac{1}{11}$.


Let $X$ denote the number of sixes that have shown up.

Then: $$\Pr(X=2\mid X\geq1)=\frac{\Pr(X=2\wedge X\geq1)}{\Pr(X\geq1)}=\frac{\Pr(X=2)}{1-\Pr(X=0)}=\frac{\frac16\frac16}{1-\frac56\frac56}=\frac1{11}$$

Sister did not say something like: "the red (or blue) die shows a six". That would have made things different. In that case $\frac16$ is the correct answer.